Start main page content

The Science Behind Project Plains Zebra

The current project is based on the hypothesis that the plains zebras are the leaders of the migrations of large herbivores on the African savannah. The conceptualization and support for this hypothesis is derived from five lines of evidence: (1) The plains zebras are part of all the migrations, which is not the case for other migratory species; (2) The plains zebras have very large non-migratory home ranges, an order of magnitude larger than most other species; (3)

The plains zebras are known to arrive at any given location first during the migration, eating the coarser grasses before the other species arrive; (4) Modelling studies indicate that the plains zebras appear to “remember” their migratory route; and (5) The neural navigation system within the plains zebra brain is the largest of all mammals, indeed all vertebrates, investigated to date. It is this combination of evidence that allows the development of the hypothesis that the plains zebras lead the migrations on the African savannah. By extension, the plains zebras are the keystone species on the African savannah and need to be the focus of intensive study, as this will lead to a deep understanding of the functioning of this globally important ecosystem and allow for pragmatic plans for its preservation to be developed.

Plains Zebras are part of all mass migrations

While the African savannah is a vast region, encompassing almost 5% of the Earth’s terrestrial surface, at present we only have validated records of 4 mass migrations of large mammals across this ecosystem, although there are likely to be many more. These annual migrations of large herbivorous mammals are essential to the ecosystem’s health as these migrations move resources through the ecosystem, and while the Serengeti-Mara migration is very well-documented (e.g., Homewood et al., 2001), migrations also are known to occur in South Sudan (Morjan et al., 2018) and Botswana (Naidoo et al., 2016).

The one large mammal species that is a participant in all the known migrations occurring on the African savannah is the plains zebra: with the blue wildebeest, Grant’s and Thomson’s gazelles in the Serengeti-Mara (Homewood et al., 2001), the white-eared kob, tiang antelope and mongalla gazelles in two separate migrations in South Sudan (Cave and Cruikshank, 1940) and primarily by themselves in Botswana (Naidoo et al., 2016), with anecdotal reports indicate that blue wildebeest may also be involved in this migration. In addition, it has been noted that while the plains zebra present with a substantially smaller population than other migrating species, they appear to be consistently located on the flanks of the migrating herds (Cave and Cruikshank, 1940), indicating a role in directing the other migrating animals (link to video). Thus, we can reasonably conclude that, despite being only a small proportion of all migratory animals, these observations provide circumstantial evidence that the plains zebras are the essential species that allow for these migrations to occur in this ecosystem.

Non-migratory home range sizes

 When compared to all other equids, the plains zebras have by far the largest non-migratory reported home ranges (see Table below), encompassing areas from 100 to 500 km2. In contrast, other equids have home ranges an order of magnitude smaller (see Table below) (Skinner and Chimimba, 2005; Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011). If we compare the non-migratory home ranges of sympatric migrating species including blue wildebeest, Grant’s gazelle, and Thomson’s gazelle, we again see that their non-migratory home ranges are an order of magnitude smaller than those of the plains zebra. Unfortunately, the non-migratory home ranges of white-eared kob, tiang antelopes, and Mongalla gazelles are not known. Focussing on non-migrating sympatric species such as the impala, waterbuck, nyala, and kudu have home ranges an order of magnitude smaller than that of the plains zebra, while the nomadic eland has home ranges of up to 360 km2 (Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011).

Thus, the plains zebra is distinct to other equids, and indeed sympatric migrating and non-migrating artiodactyls, by having a non-migratory home range an order of magnitude larger. Thus, the large non-migrating home range and the annual migration, both support the notion that the plains zebra are the most navigationally competent species, and that they enable the migrations of large herbivores on the African savannah.

Table summarizing the differences in home range of the phylogenetically related Perissodactyls, sympatric migrating Artiodactyls, and sympatric non-migrating Artiodactyls with the plains zebra. Note, that apart from the nomadic eland, the home range of the plains zebra is an order of magnitude larger than the other species.

Common name

Scientific name

Home range (km2)

Source

Perissodactyls

Plains zebra

Equus quagga

100-500

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Grevy’s zebra

Equus grevyi

10

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Cape Mountain zebra

Equus zebra zebra

3-16

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Hartmann’s mountain zebra

Equus zebra hartmannae

6-20

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Namibian wild horse

Equus ferus caballus

34

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Przewalski’s horse

Equus ferus przewalskii

2-11

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

African wild ass

Equus africanus

20

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Sympatric migrating Artiodactyls

blue wildebeest

Connochaetes taurinus

10

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Grant’s gazelle

Nanger granti

20

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

Thomson’s gazelle

Eudorcas thomsonii

10

Skinner and Chimimba, 2005

white-eared kob

Kobus leucotis

unknown

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

tiang antelopes

Damaliscus tiang

unknown

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

Mongalla gazelles

Eudorcas albonotata

unknown

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

Sympatric non- migrating Artiodactyls

impala

Aepyceros melampus

3

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

waterbuck

Kobus ellipsiprymnus

7

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

nyala

Tragelaphus angasii

10

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

kudu

Tragelaphus strepsiceros

3

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

eland

Taurotragus oryx

360

Wilson and Mittermeier, 2011

Grazing succession studies

Grazing succession studies of the migratory populations have shown that the first species to arrive at any specific location on the migratory paths are the plains zebras, followed by other migratory species in the subsequent days and weeks (Vesey-Fitzgerald, 1960; Gwynne and Bell, 1968; Anderson et al., 2024).

The proposed reason for this temporal migratory sequence is the selective grazing of the plains zebra on the tallest and coarsest grasses (stems), which then exposes and makes more readily accessible the shorter dicotyledon material selectively grazed upon by other migratory species (Vesey-Fitzgerald, 1960; Gwynne and Bell, 1968), or that plains zebras are “nudged” into leading the migrations through competitive grazing (Anderson et al., 2024). This indicates that for the migrations to be successful, the plains zebras must arrive first, otherwise the foliage required for other migratory species will not be exposed and would be more difficult to access.

These observations provide further evidence that the plains zebras, out of necessity, lead the migrations.

Plains zebras appear to remember their migratory route

A modelling study of the trajectories that plains zebra traverse for migration in Botswana indicated that memory, rather than sensory cues, are what are guiding the migrations (Bracis and Mueller, 2017). In this study it was shown that it is more likely that the plains zebras use their experience of previous resource availability to determine their migratory path, rather than using sensory cues to guide their long-range movements. By using their memory of the migration rather than sensory cues, the modelled plains zebras arrived closer to their migration destination when compared to modelled plains zebras that relied on sensory cues. One prediction that could be made from this modelling study is that the neural navigation system that directs the movements of the plains zebra might be specialized, and this is indeed what we have observed.

Hippocampal size and navigation

Plains zebras brains have an average mass of 537 grams, a mass that one would predict given their body mass (250-350 kg). Most of the brain of the plains zebra appears very similar to what one would see in their close relatives the mountain zebra and domestic horse. The one striking difference we have noted is that the hippocampal formation of the plains zebra is unusually large – in both absolute and relative terms. The absolute volume of the hippocampus in the plains zebra is 10.77 ml, being substantially larger than the 5.19 ml observed in the closely related mountain zebra and the 4.43 ml observed in the domestic horse, clearly exceeding what might be expected from a species with an average brain mass of 537 g compared to the 433 g brain mass of the mountain zebra and the 515 g brain mass of the domestic horse. The hippocampal volume of the plains zebra rivals that of the African elephant (10.84 ml), but with a brain mass of 4900 g the African elephant hippocampus is relatively smaller than that of the plains zebra. Hippocampal volumes reported for humans range from 8.35-8.46 ml (Maguire, 2000, 2006) up to 10.29 ml, thus the size of the plains zebra hippocampus exceeds that of humans, and given that human brains weigh around 1400 g, exceeds the human in size relative to brain mass. Indeed the hippocampus in the plains zebra is three times larger than we would expect for its brain mass.

In addition to this large size, our preliminary studies of the internal organization of the hippocampal formation of the plains zebra revealed a potential internal specialisation. While possessing the typical features of the hippocampal formation (such as entorhinal cortex, subicular complex, cornu ammonis regions CA1-4, and the dentate gyrus), one specific part, CA1, occupies a larger proportion of the hippocampal formation and has approximately 1.64 times the number of neurons in this region than seen in closely related species.

The CA1 region, especially the dorsal part, is of specific interest, as it is within this region that the “place cells” crucial for neural navigation are located. While the study of the plains zebra hippocampus requires a great deal more to be undertaken, what we can conclude from what we have observed is that it appears that a true cognitive specialization, related to navigation, has evolved in the plains zebra. This will provide the neural backing the plains zebras require to lead the migrations and the capacity to remember the migratory path and all associated landmarks needed to navigate the several hundred kilometres they traverse each year.

Our preliminary results indicate that the plains zebra is unique in this sense, with sympatric migratory species, such as the blue wildebeest, and non-migratory closely related species such as the mountain zebra, having hippocampal formations that could be considered quite typical for these species.

Sources
  • Anderson T, Hepler S, Holdo R, Donaldson J, Erhardt R, Hopcraft J, Hutchinson M, Huebner S, Morrison T, Muday J, Munuo I, Palmer M, Pansu J, Pringle R, Sketch R, Packer C (2024) Interplay of competition and facilitation in grazing succession by migrant Serengeti herbivores. Science 383:782-788.
  • Bracis C, Mueller T (2017) Memory, not just perception, plays and important role in terrestrial mammalian migration. Proc R Soc B 284:2017-449
  • Cave FO, Cruikshank A (1940) A note on game migration in the south eastern Sudan. Sudan Notes and Records 23:341-344.
  • Gwynne MD, Bell RHV (1968). Selection of vegetation components by grazing ungulates in the Serengeti National Park. Nature 220: 390-393
  • Homewood K, Lambin EF, Coast E, Kariuki A, Kikula I, Kikula J, Said M, Serneels S, Thompson M (2001) Long-term changes in Serengeti-Mara wildebeest and land cover: pastoralism, population, or policies? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:12544-12549.
  • Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:4398-4403.
  • Maguire EA, Woollett K, Spiers HJ (2006) London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus 16:1091-1101.
  • Morjan MD, Rayl ND, Elkan PW, Deutsch JC, Henke MB, Fuller TK (2018) Armed conflict and development in South Sudan threatens some of Africa’s longest and largest ungulate migrations. Biodiversity and Conservation 27:365-380.
  • Naidoo R, Chase MJ, Beytell P, du Preez P, Landen K, Stuart-Hill G, Taylor R (2016) A newly discovered wildlife migration in Namibia and Botswana is the longest in Africa. Oryx 50:138-146.
  • Skinner and Chimimba (2005) The Mammals of the Southern African Sub-region. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
  • Vesey-Fitzgerald DF (1960) Grazing succession among East African game animals. Journal of Mammalogy, 41:161-172
  • Wilson DE Mittermeier RA eds (2011) Handbook of the Mammals of the World Volume 2, Hoofed mammals. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona.
Share